University of Nebraska – Lincoln
The relationship of situated social
cognition factors and consent to search perceptions in innocent suspects
{ Discussion }
The results of this research were surprising, especially in relation
to the room size variable and the differences across dependent variables. To
begin, we hypothesized that gender differences would appear across the various
conditions, and this turned out to be the case. While the main effect of gender
on freedom to refuse consent was only marginally significant (p=.08), pairwise comparisons did reveal
small differences between genders in specific conditions. In fact, it seems as
though the SSC manipulations did little to nothing in relation to females’
freedom to refuse consent scores, as there were no differences between cell
means, regardless of condition. For men, however, there were numerous
differences: in the small room, males gave higher ratings in the lit condition
than in the dark; in the lit condition, males gave higher ratings in the small
room than in the large; and there were main effects for both lighting and room
size that matched these patterns. These findings may be related to the unequal
sample sizes between the two genders. Further research should investigate these
gender differences to determine if this is the case or another factor is
playing a role to moderate or mediate the relationship. Gender may be acting as
a proxy.
Next, we hypothesized room size would be related to both
voluntariness of consent and freedom to refuse consent, such that those in the
larger room would give higher ratings of both voluntariness and freedom to
refuse. Cesario and colleagues (2010) found that, in
confrontational situations, “fight” responses are more likely in enclosed
spaces, while “flight” responses are more likely in open fields. This finding,
however, did not appear in our research. There was a main effect for room size
such that higher ratings of freedom to refuse consent were given in the small
room than in the large room, opposite of that which we hypothesized. It is not
clear why this relationship materialized and, again, it was only descriptive
for the males. Additional studies can examine room size in other situations to
see how and if the effects replicate.
Because there has been little to no research on the influence of
lighting as an SSC manipulation, we hypothesized that the lit condition would
be analogous to the open area condition in Cesario
and colleagues’ study (2010), with higher ratings of freedom to refuse consent
being related to the lit condition. The effects of lighting did follow this
hypothesized pattern, though they were then conflicting with the effects of
room size. Again, there was no effect for females, but there was a main effect for
lighting for males (with higher ratings in the lit condition). This effect was
descriptive in the small room; there was no difference by lighting condition in
the large room. This was also surprising. Brightness was balanced across room
conditions with differing light bulb wattage, so the total output of light did
not vary with room size in that regard.
Finally, we hypothesized that those in the lit and large room
condition would give the highest ratings for both dependent variables, so that
the two-way interaction between the SSC variables and freedom to refuse consent
would yield a larger effect than the other two-way interactions. This was
indeed the case, though the F-test
was just larger than the gender by lighting interaction. Again, though, the effect
was only partially in the hypothesized direction: the small and lit room
yielded the highest ratings of freedom to refuse consent. Overall, the main
effect of lighting was the strongest effect in the analysis (F=6.22, p=.01).
We anticipated that the relationships would be similar between the
independent variables and each dependent variable. It had been assumed that
“voluntariness of consent” and “freedom to refuse consent” were similar enough
to yield analogous results. However, this was not the case. Although nearly
every effect significantly related to the former outcome variable, very few
related to the latter. While the three-way interaction was the second largest
effect for freedom to refuse consent, it had no relationship to voluntariness
of consent. For this reason, age was exploratorily
investigated, too. The resultant four-way interaction between gender, room
size, lighting condition, and age was in fact significant. Apart from this
effect, only the two-way interactions between gender and room size and gender
and lighting were significant. While the former analyses demonstrated the
relationships that SSC variables can have with decision perceptions, the
voluntariness of consent analyses lean toward the importance of the demographic
variables.
Interestingly, there continued to be no differences in ratings of
voluntariness between conditions within the large room condition. In the small
and lit room, there were differences between age and gender: older males gave
higher ratings than younger males and younger females gave higher ratings than
older females. These findings may speak to processing differences between males
and females or may be due to the age cutoffs we used in discriminating between
older and younger students. The distribution is roughly even between younger
(18-19, likely underclassmen) and older (20-24, likely upperclassmen) students.
Additional analyses using other age groups or through a continuous variable and
ANCOVA may add to the findings presented here.
There was again an effect for lighting for males in the small
room, but it is now limited just to those males in the older condition. This
group may have also been driving the effect in the freedom to refuse consent
interaction. No other effects were found with voluntariness of consent as the
dependent variable. As previously stated, we had assumed that these outcome
variables were of relatively equal valence, but that appears not to be the
case. As such, vocabulary is another factor that may need to be taken into
account, as it relates to the justice system. The independent variables are
included herein to discuss their relationship to perceptions of a search
request to help inform courts what safeguards ought to be in place to protect
their constituents against unlawful searches and seizures. However, the
differences between “voluntariness” and “freedom to refuse” may attest that not
just the judicial branch, but also the legislative branch has a role in
dictating consent validity. The vernacular used in legislation pertaining to
searches may alter perceptions of such search requests and, in turn, play a
role in litigation related to the legality of search requests. Thus, it must
also be included in the “totality of the circumstances.”
Future initiatives should acquire a more diverse and balanced
sample; the sample used here lacked equality in both race and gender,
categories with groups that likely differ in important ways. Other SSC
variables should be included and tested in innovative ways to add to the
literature and further examine how environmental influences relate to
decision-making and perceptions of such decisions. Likewise, other outcome
variables may yield different results, as shown here. The wording of the
variables may be important and weighed appropriately. Finally, the results of
this research show that certain search request characteristics may be more
important than others may and such interpretations may vary by group. Thus,
all, and likely others, should be considered when evaluating the nature of a
search request.
Home | Introduction | Method | Results | Discussion | Tables & Figures
| References | PDF