University of Nebraska – Lincoln
The relationship of situated social
cognition factors and consent to search perceptions in innocent suspects
{ Results }
Between-groups factorial ANOVAs were conducted to examine the relationships
between situated social cognition predictors, gender, and perceptions of the
search request and the participants’ consent. Table 1 displays a summary of the
descriptive statistics.
Freedom to refuse consent to the search request
The results yielded a significant three-way interaction between
gender, room size, and lighting conditions, as they relate to freedom to refuse
consent to the search request, F(1,256)=5.53,
MSe=5.50, p=.02, r=.15. Table 2
displays all F-tests. Estimated marginal
means were used to compare the cell means to determine the direction of the
interaction. The results were such that for males, those in the lit condition
reported higher ratings of freedom to refuse consent when in the small room
(5.67>2.68, p<.01, r=.54), a finding contrary to the
research hypothesis that those in the large room would give higher ratings.
Males in the dark condition reported no difference in levels of freedom to
refuse consent between the small and large room conditions (2.68=3.21, p=.49), also contrary to that which was
hypothesized. Likewise, for females, there was no difference in ratings of
freedom to refuse consent by room size for either the lit (3.25=3.12, p=.78) or dark condition (3.20=2.95, p=.62).
The factorial ANOVA also yielded a significant two-way interaction
for the situated social cognition factors—room size and lighting—as they relate
to freedom to refuse consent to the search request, F(1,256)=4.72, MSe=5.50, p=.03, r=.14. This effect showed that those in the small room gave higher
ratings of freedom to refuse consent in the lit condition than in the dark
condition (4.46>2.94, p<.01,
r=.31), as was hypothesized. However, there was no difference in ratings
between the lit and dark conditions in the large room (3.18=3.08, p=.82). This overall interaction is
descriptive for males but not for females, as females had no difference in
freedom to refuse consent ratings between lit and dark conditions in the small
room. We also hypothesized that the interaction between room size and lighting
would yield the highest ratings. This effect is indeed the largest of the
two-way interactions and is only smaller than the three-way interaction and
main effect for lighting.
There is also a significant two-way interaction between gender and
lighting, F(1,256)=4.62, MSe=5.50, p=.03, r=.13, such that
males in the lit condition gave higher ratings of freedom to refuse consent
than did those in the dark condition (4.46>2.95, p<.01, r=.31). This
effect supports the research hypothesis that those in the lit condition would
give higher ratings than those in the dark condition. The effect was
descriptive for males in the small room, as hypothesized, but not for males in
the large room, for whom there was no difference between the lighting conditions
in ratings of freedom to refuse consent (3.25=3.21, p=.80). For females, however, there was no difference in ratings of
freedom to consent between the lit and dark conditions (3.18=3.07, p=.74), an effect that was descriptive
for both room size conditions.
The two-way interaction between gender and room size is not
significant, F(1,256)=1.345, MSe=5.50, p=.25, although this is not descriptive, as there is a marginally
significant effect for males, such that those in the small room gave higher
ratings of freedom to refuse consent than did those in the large room
(4.18>3.23, p=.09, r=.19). There was no effect for females
(3.22=3.04, p=.58), for whom the (lack of) interaction is descriptive.
There is also a main effect of lighting, F(1,256)=6.22, MSe=5.50, p=.01, r=.15, such that those in the lit condition gave higher ratings of
freedom to refuse consent than those in the dark condition, in support of the
research hypothesis. This main effect is descriptive for males in the small
room (5.67>2.68, p<.01, r=.54), but is misleading in all other
conditions, for which there are no significant effects.
The main effects of gender and room size were both marginally
significant, F(1,256)=3.12, MSe=5.50, p=.08, r=.11 and F(1,256)=3.05, MSe=5.50, p=.08, r=.11, respectively. Males gave higher
ratings of freedom to refuse consent than did females. This effect was
descriptive for participants in the small room in the lit condition
(5.67>3.25, p<.01, r=.46), but is misleading in all other
conditions. The main effect of room size shows that those in the small room
gave higher freedom to refuse consent ratings than did those in the large room,
opposite of the research hypothesis and that which Cesario and colleagues
(2010) would have predicted. However, this effect was only descriptive for
males in the lit condition (5.67>3.25, p<.01,
r=.46), and is misleading in all
other conditions.
Voluntariness of consent to the search request
The same three-way interaction examined voluntariness of consent
to the search request as the dependent variable. (Refer to Table 3.) However,
the three-way interaction was non-significant with few significant lower-order
effects. We exploratorily added age as a fourth independent variable to analyze
if a four-way interaction would be viable. In fact, the four-way interaction
was significant, F(1,256)=4.22, MSe=6.75, p=.04, r=.13. Follow-up
analyses of the estimated marginal means of the cells revealed that older males
in the small room and lit condition gave higher ratings of voluntariness of
consent than did younger males in the same conditions (7.80>4.50, p<.01, r=.54). There was no difference between older and younger males in
the small and dark room. Furthermore, there were no significant effects for
males or females in the large rooms, regardless of age or lighting condition.
Although, there was a marginally significant effect for females in the small
and lit room condition, such that younger females gave higher ratings of
voluntariness of consent than did older females (4.74>3.42, p=.08, r=.25). However, there was no difference in voluntariness ratings
between younger and older females in the dark, small room condition.
As previously stated, the original three-way analysis of gender,
room size, and lighting condition was not significant, F(1,256)=1.226, MSe=6.75,
p=.27. Similarly, none of the
additional three-way interactions involving age was significant.
Of the two-way interactions, one was significant and another was
marginally significant, though the other four did not achieve statistical significance.
There is an interaction between gender and lighting, as they relate to
perceptions of voluntariness of consent to the search, F(1,256)=3.90, MSe=6.75, p<.05, r=.12. This interaction showed that males gave marginally higher
ratings of voluntariness of consent in the lit condition than did females
(5.19>4.25, p=.07, r=.18). However, this effect is only
descriptive for males in the small, lit, and older condition, compared to
females in the same condition (7.80>3.42, p<.01, r=.64). There
is no difference at any other level of the lit conditions. In addition, this
two-way interaction shows there is no difference between the genders in the
dark condition (411=4.65, p=.32),
which is descriptive at all levels of the dark condition.
There is a marginally significant two-way interaction for gender
and room size, as they relate to voluntariness of consent to the search
request, F(1,256)=3.68, MSe=6.75, p=.06, r=.12. This interaction
shows males rate their voluntariness of consent as marginally higher than do
females when both are in the small room condition (5.26>4.34, p=.08, r=.17). This interaction is descriptive for older males in the
small and lit room, as compared to older females in the same condition
(7.80>3.42, p<.01, r=.64), but is misleading at all other
levels of the small room condition. However, for this interaction, there is no
difference by gender in voluntariness ratings when in the large room condition
(4.04=4.56, p=.33), which is
descriptive across all levels of the large room condition. No other two-way
interaction or any of the main effects was significant.
Home | Introduction | Method | Results | Discussion | Tables & Figures
| References | PDF